Summary: Researchers discovered children’s ability to process emotional cues is largely established by the time they start school. Analyzing brain scans from children aged five to 15, the study revealed that such brain activity becomes increasingly similar to their peers as they reach adolescence. The research emphasizes the importance of early intervention to address issues related to understanding emotional cues.
Key Facts:
- The research team analyzed brain scan data from 823 children, who were shown emotionally nuanced videos while in an MRI scanner. The distinct brain activity patterns corresponding to each emotion were noted.
- The study provides evidence that the optimal time for intervention is during early childhood to support a child’s social and emotional development better.
- The study’s lead author, Dr. M. Catalina Camacho, is involved in a pilot study to explore how these emotion-specific activation patterns emerge in early childhood among children at high risk for anxiety. She believes such information can inform relationship and parenting-focused interventions.
Children with specific psychiatric symptoms, such as anxiety, may struggle to understand emotional cues when people around them are happy, sad or angry, and that lack of understanding can make it difficult to respond appropriately in social situations.
Studying brain scans from hundreds of children ages five to 15, researchers at Washington University School of Medicine in St. Louis have found that how children’s brains process emotional cues typically is mostly set by the time they are school age. And as they reach adolescence, such brain activity becomes similar to the way the brains of their peers process such cues.
The research provides additional evidence that the best time to intervene to address issues children may have in reading others’ emotional cues is early, even before they start school.
“It appears that activation patterns in the brain for processing naturalistic emotional cues are pretty well set by the time a child reaches school age,” said M. Catalina Camacho, Ph.D., a postdoctoral researcher in the Department of Psychiatry at Washington University and the study’s first author.
“While the patterns become more refined in adolescence, they don’t change substantially. What that means is that when the response to others’ emotions is unusual—as it can be in anxiety, autism or depression—we really need to be intervening during early childhood to better support the child’s social and emotional development.”
Camacho and her colleagues analyzed brain scan data from 823 kids who were shown two videos while in a magnetic resonance imaging scanner. One was a Pixar short entitled “The Present,” in which a young boy who is missing part of one of his legs is given the gift of a puppy who also is missing part of a leg.
The other video was a 10-minute scene from the animated film “Despicable Me,” in which a supervillain begins a family by adopting three orphan girls, and he must choose whether he is more committed to being a villain or to raising his daughters.
“We were surprised by the findings because the brain activity for each emotion was so distinct among these children,” Camacho said.
“Overall, the activation patterns seem to be very well set for each emotion category, which tells me we need to study even younger kids to determine when these brain responses begin to take shape.”
Similar studies that identify others’ emotions have been conducted in adults and older children using more static stimuli.
“Adults and older kids know that when they see a picture of a sad face, they’re meant to interpret that the person is sad, but if you show the same picture to a 3-year-old, they often won’t recognize the emotion because there’s no context showing why the person is making a sad expression,” Camacho explained.
“With movies, we can bridge that gap because movies present cues with much-needed context about the emotions.”
Perceiving and identifying emotions in others is a complex process that involves nearly every cognitive network in the brain.
“Many psychiatric conditions, such as anxiety and various personality disorders, are associated with difficulty reading others’ emotions, or less empathy,” said senior investigator Deanna M. Barch, Ph.D., the Gregory B. Couch Professor of Psychiatry and head of the Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences.
“We didn’t actually measure empathy, but we were able to see how children’s brains encode emotional cues from others, which is an important component of empathy.”
For this study, the researchers analyzed data from the Healthy Brain Network study, a New York-based imaging study in which the brains of children are scanned to look at how various brain structures and networks develop. Data from the study is publicly available to scientists all over the world.
In follow-up work, Camacho is involved in a pilot study to map how these emotion-specific activation patterns emerge across early childhood in children at high risk for anxiety.
She believes that such information can inform relationship and parenting-focused interventions to help parents and children better read one another’s emotional cues as a way to support better emotional development.
“These sorts of interventions might represent one way to help kids develop the ability to read other people’s emotions and respond appropriately,” she said.
Abstract
Large-scale encoding of emotion concepts becomes increasingly similar between individuals from childhood to adolescence
Humans require a shared conceptualization of others’ emotions for adaptive social functioning.
A concept is a mental blueprint that gives our brains parameters for predicting what will happen next. Emotion concepts undergo refinement with development, but it is not known whether their neural representations change in parallel.
Here, in a sample of 5–15-year-old children (n = 823), we show that the brain represents different emotion concepts distinctly throughout the cortex, cerebellum and caudate. Patterns of activation to each emotion changed little across development.
Using a model-free approach, we show that activation patterns were more similar between older children than between younger children. Moreover, scenes that required inferring negative emotional states elicited higher default mode network activation similarity in older children than younger children.
These results suggest that representations of emotion concepts are relatively stable by mid to late childhood and synchronize between individuals during adolescence.
About this emotion and neurodevelopment research news
Author: Jim Dryden
Source: WUSTL
Contact: Jim Dryden – WUSTL
Original Research: Closed access.
“Large-scale encoding of emotion concepts becomes increasingly similar between individuals from childhood to adolescence” by M. Catalina Camacho et al. Nature Neuroscience